the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 License.
the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 License.

Review article: A scoping review of human factors in avalanche decision-making
Rebecca A. Hetland
Tarjei Tveito Skille
Andrea Mannberg
The interest in understanding the human aspects of avalanche risk mitigation has steadily grown over the past few decades. Between 2001–2011, 11 research papers on decision-making in avalanche terrain were published in peer- reviewed journals. Between 2012–2022, this number rose to 55. These papers have been authored by researchers from various disciplines and publications in journals across different fields. Despite the field's nascent stage, to guide future research, it is pertinent to provide an overview of the insights from the existing research literature.
This paper offers a systematic overview of peer-reviewed research on human factors in avalanche decision-making. The overview is based on a systematic literature search covering research published up until the end of 2022. The search was conducted across six databases, including Scopus and Web of Science, using a set of keywords related to avalanche decision-making (e.g., “decision-making,” “backcountry skiing,” “avalanche terrain,” “avalanche accident”). Out of nearly 13 000 articles containing at least one of the key search terms, 70 had a research question related to avalanche decision-making and were published in peer-reviewed academic journals. Additionally, 81 relevant papers were published as ISSW (International Snow Science Workshop) proceedings.
We coded all the identified papers based on major and minor research questions, control variables, population covered, and methodology. Twelve concepts described the different research themes (e.g., avalanche accidents, avalanche education, decision-making strategies). Due to a large variation in quality regarding the ISSW papers, we only applied these concepts to the 70 peer-reviewed papers and present them by their main concept. The extracted data from all papers including the ISSW papers can be found at https://osf.io/u9ydm/ (last access: 12 February 2025).
- Article
(747 KB) - Full-text XML
- BibTeX
- EndNote
1.1 Rationale
Approximately 90 % of fatal snow avalanche accidents are triggered by the victim or someone in their group (Schweizer and Lütschg, 2001). This underscores the point that avalanches are more of a human issue than a snow issue.
Over the past 2 decades, there has been a growing body of research focusing on what has been labeled “human factors”. The role of human factors has previously been extensively researched in a range of other scientific fields, e.g., economics, geography, outdoors and recreation, political science, psychology, and public safety and engineering research. It should be noted that the exact definition of the term human factors differs across different disciplines. Within the avalanche research field, human factors have been defined to encompass any human influences that affect the assessment of avalanche risks and the decision-making process (Haegeli et al., 2023). However, even within this literature, different research traditions offer different approaches, thus creating a body of knowledge that is heterogeneous in nature. To create a more informative and productive foundation for future research on human factors in avalanche decision-making, we conducted a scoping review.
1.2 Objectives
By conducting a scoping review, we wished to examine the extent, range, and nature of the evidence so far produced on human factors in avalanche terrain. The following research question has guided this effort: what literature exists on how human factors affect decision-making and/or risk assessment performed by individuals who expose themselves to avalanche-prone terrain?
The main objectives of our research were
- a.
to design and implement a systematic literature search on the topic of human factors in avalanche terrain,
- b.
to identify relevant literature and extract data from the papers to create a detailed overview of this literature.
2.1 Scoping review
A scoping review is a type of knowledge synthesis that follows a systematic approach to map evidence on a topic and identify the main concepts, theories, sources, and knowledge gaps (Tricco et al., 2018). Unlike systematic reviews, which typically address narrowly focused research questions, scoping reviews cover broader topics and are often used to identify and analyze the extent, range, and nature of research activity in a particular field. By choosing this approach and following the guidance of the PRISMA-ScR checklist, we wished to summarize findings from a body of knowledge that is heterogeneous in both its methods and the disciplines involved and to reveal uncharted research areas within the avalanche research field.
2.2 Eligibility criteria
Our guiding principle has been that human factors must be central in the included papers. We identified literature where human factors influence actual decision-making or risk assessment not only while exposed in avalanche terrain, but also in the preparation phase before entering avalanche terrain. Preparation may include both trip planning and avalanche education (Greene et al., 2022). Literature focused on decision-making tools was considered relevant in cases where use of the tool is related to human factors in decision-making but not where the focus is on how the tool relates to weather, terrain, and snowpack aspects. In the following paragraphs, we will elaborate and rationalize our criteria for inclusion and exclusion.
2.2.1 Publication status
Human factors in avalanche terrain are a nascent research field that has attracted large interest among practitioners, stakeholders and users of avalanche terrain. A relatively large share of the literature consists of papers that are not published peer-reviewed papers (grey literature) but mainly proceedings from the International Snow Science Workshop (ISSW) or undergraduate and graduate theses (BA, MSc, PhD). The PRISMA guidelines are open to including grey literature, and we initially planned to include this ourselves. Since such works have not gone through a peer-review process, we created an additional set of inclusion criteria where we only included non-peer-reviewed papers that (1) contained a clear research question or objective, (2) presented a description of the method used to answer the research question or reach the objective, (3) built on previous research (i.e., included at least one reference to peer-reviewed research), and (4) did not have a peer-reviewed duplicate. However, our analysis of the papers revealed a substantial spread in quality even after applying these criteria. While some papers would perhaps have been accepted for publication with only minor revisions after peer review, others would likely have been given a desk reject. This made it very difficult to develop stringent inclusion criteria. Admittedly, there is also a spread in quality in peer-reviewed articles, but the spread in the grey literature is much larger, and since conducting detailed reviews of the quality of the papers is outside of the scope of this paper, we decided to exclude all grey literature. The avalanche research field is different from other research fields because many practitioners undertake important research that they present at the ISSW but never even try to publish in peer-reviewed formats. The ISSW conference proceedings are of special importance in this field. We therefore searched through and extracted data from all the 81 ISSW papers that passed the grey-literature criteria and organized them thematically in the same way as we did for the peer-reviewed papers. The results can be found at https://osf.io/u9ydm/ (last access: 12 February 2025).
2.2.2 Participants
All people exposed to avalanche terrain in the backcountry, sidecountry, or out-of-bounds terrain were considered eligible research participants in the included sources of evidence. This includes participants maneuvering avalanche terrain by snowmobiles, snowboard, snowshoes, skis, and foot. Recreationists, professional guides, avalanche safety instructors and educators, ski area patrollers, avalanche professionals (observers, bulletin makers, investigators), and other personnel that are expected to personally mitigate and consider avalanche risk (e.g., field geologists, trained soldiers) were included as participants. People appearing as participants through accident reports were also included in the review, as profile information of avalanche victims is considered important information on how human factors may have played a vital role in the decision-making process prior to the avalanche accident. Traveling into avalanche terrain might be self-assisted, snowmobile-assisted, lift-assisted, or motor-vehicle-assisted (e.g., helicopter, snowcats).
People traveling by vehicle on roads exposed to avalanche terrain were not included in this review. The rationale behind this is that decisions concerning road risk and safety are made by official authorities and not by the individuals themselves. Residents living in avalanche-exposed areas were excluded from our study according to the same rationale.
2.2.3 Years considered
In order to include pioneering research and publications that have contributed formatively to the development of the field, we did not set a lower limit on the publication year. Our search was run up until the end of 2022.
2.2.4 Language
Our study has limited its inclusion to sources written in English.
2.2.5 Exclusion criteria
We chose to exclude research that focuses strictly on (1) avalanche rescue and medical issues; (2) technical aspects of weather, terrain, avalanche dynamics, and forecasting; and (3) management of operations where the decision-maker is not personally affected by the avalanche threat (like risk management in a ski resort). Our rationale for excluding these important fields is that these research areas do not analyze how individuals personally deal with the threat of being involved in an avalanche accident. We also excluded articles where humans and human behavior in avalanche terrain is secondary or implied as part of the research (e.g., extensive accident reports, outdoor or adventure focus). Topics such as decision-making related to rescue after an avalanche has occurred, including medical issues, were not included in the search. Natural science studies or studies primarily focusing on building or technical aspects of avalanche forecasting were also not included. However, we note that we did include studies that investigated the effect of avalanche forecast on human factors. Finally, we excluded sources of evidence where the full text was not obtainable or where human factors were auxiliary or briefly mentioned but were not among the main themes. The excluded topics are also of interest to the scientific community but will require separate searches and are not within the scope of this review.
2.3 Information sources
We defined six databases and search engines as relevant to our topic “human factors in avalanche terrain”. As the topic is not easily restricted to a specific discipline, Web of Science and Scopus were considered useful sources. They both offer access to multiple databases that reference cross-disciplinary research. Two other discipline specific databases, PsycInfo and Hospitality & Tourism Complete, were chosen because of the assumption that human factors in avalanche terrain would be published in these academic disciplines. Our previous knowledge of the existing literature led us to this assumption. In addition, we also ran the search in the ISSW proceedings database and ProQuest – a database covering dissertations from a range of disciplines. The results from the latter two, primarily originating from the ISSW database, have been subject to the same procedure as the peer-reviewed articles presented in this paper. The results, included the extracted data, can be found in supporting materials (see https://osf.io/u9ydm/). Google Scholar was used as a tool in preliminary searches and to supplement the final search. We conducted the search between 27 April 2017 and 31 December 2022. Where sources of evidence were found as references or abstracts but with missing full texts, effort was made to retrieve these texts by requests to relevant libraries or by contacting authors.
2.4 Search
2.4.1 Identifying relevant keywords for systematic search
We identified keywords using an iterative process. In the first phase, we searched Google Scholar using intuitive search words such as “human factor in avalanche terrain”. We thereafter used the relevant keywords in the identified papers in a second systematic search: “The Human Factor in Avalanche Terrain”.
The keywords and phrases chosen for our search were first selected based on their frequency in the keywords overview (see “keyword, selection.docx” for more details). Other keywords were added after consulting with researchers familiar with the field. We ran several preliminary searches in the named databases to refine the final set of keywords. The quantity of the search results has been used as a guide to define the relevance and usefulness of the keywords.
2.4.2 Building the search
We created two bins, (1) human factor and (2) avalanche. These two bins have a list of associated keywords. Any paper with keywords that matched both bins would be listed as a result. The search is built using the Boolean operators OR and AND, where OR is used between all the keywords within the main categories and AND is used to combine the two categories for the final result. We searched for keywords in titles, abstracts, and listed keywords. Thesaurus terms (predefined keywords for specific databases) were added to the databases with this functionality. Table 1 provides an overview of relevant categories of keywords in the two bins (for more details see “Identifying keywords.docx” and “Keywords, overview.docx” at https://osf.io/u9ydm/).
2.4.3 Selection of sources of evidence
The final search result from the individual databases and search engines were added to our library, and duplicates were filtered out. Guided by our research objectives and eligibility criteria, preliminary screening was performed based on the title and abstract, separating obviously ineligible studies from possible eligible ones. We used a folder structure that categorized sources as included, uncertain, and excluded. In the next step, two researchers read the full text. Notes were subsequently compared, and in cases where there was disagreement, the papers were discussed in depth and a conclusion was drawn based on the extent to which they answered the research objectives and fulfilled the eligibility criteria. This process was repeated in three iterations. The final result yielded 70 peer-reviewed papers (see Fig. 1). We conducted the same process for the ISSW proceedings.
2.5 Data charting process
To extract relevant data from the papers, two of the authors developed a matrix schema for charting data from the sources of evidence included. Data were extracted on the basis of year of publication; type of publication; sampling procedure; method of data collection; type of study design; participants (e.g., self- or lift-assisted recreationists, avalanche educators, avalanche forecasters); risk target (the population at risk, e.g., recreationists, avalanche professionals); focus of study; main explanatory factor, if existing; and, if relevant, control variables of data.
Two independent researchers extracted and coded the data. Notes were subsequently compared and discussed, and if the two coders were not in agreement or if any kind of uncertainty was identifiable, a conclusion was made based on a further discussion with an extended panel of one or two researchers. Table 2 provides a description of the categories of extracted data.
Table 2Description of the categories of extracted data from the data charting. See below the table for definitions of abbreviations.

Avy denotes avalanche (e.g., avy professional denotes avalanche professional), and DM denotes decision-making. * FACETS is the acronym introduced by McCammon (2002), used to describe six key factors likely to influence our decision- making (familiarity, acceptance, consistency, expert halo, tracks (scarcity), and social facilitation).
2.5.1 Categorization of papers according to their main focus
We coded all papers according to their main focus. The different focus themes were developed using an iterative process. One of the authors suggested a first set of themes, based on a previous, non-systematic review of the literature. During the data's coding process, the two coding researchers could add themes if a paper did not fit the existing themes. In total, 20 themes were identified in the eligible material.
Organizing the literature into 20 themes provides an overview of topics covered in the literature so far. However, some of the topics identified are very narrow, and others overlap. The high number of topics may also make the overview less clear. We therefore decided to revise the codes into a smaller number of research themes. Three of this paper's authors made an initial suggestion of eight research themes. These themes were sent to three international collaborators for feedback and discussion. Based on the discussion, the themes were revised into 12 main research themes (Table 3).
Two of the authors and the three international collaborators thereafter independently assigned at least one concept to each paper in the dataset. The assignment was based on the focal research question of the article and not based on the potential relevance for a given research area. For example, studies analyzing avalanche education directly were assigned the concept “avalanche education”, while studies that might be relevant to avalanche education but did not explicitly investigate the effects of avalanche education or avalanche course curricula were not assigned this concept. Since some papers cover more than one topic, we provided each paper with up to three different concepts. In cases of disagreement, notes were compared and discussed, and concepts were adjusted.
Of the 12 995 articles that contained at least one of the keywords in the two categories, 76 fulfilled the eligibility criteria and were included in the dataset. During the analysis of the data, we discovered that six of the identified papers did not have human decision-making as their main focus. These papers were therefore removed, and the final dataset contained 70 articles.
The eligible papers have publication dates ranging from 1999 to 2022. Over half (N=56) were published in the last 10 years, and more than a quarter (N=22) have been published since 2020. Most studies (N=43) rely on quantitative methods. A relatively small number use qualitative (N=9) or mixed methods (N=11). Only three studies use randomized sampling strategies, 70 % rely on convenience samples (N=50), and only 64 % (N=46) study backcountry recreationists. The results from the data charting process with extracted data can be found at https://osf.io/u9ydm/ (last access: 12 February 2025).
3.1 Main research themes in the eligible literature
We provide a brief overview of the research themes situated based on research traditions and concepts from related research fields. The list is not meant to cover all potentially relevant research themes on the human dimension of avalanche risk. In Table 4 the papers are sorted according to the different research themes.
Table 4Eligible papers sorted according to their main research themes. One paper can be tagged in up to three research themes.

3.1.1 Biases and decision-making errors (N=11)
A range of cognitive and motivational biases can influence decision-making, including those related to risk analysis (Montibeller and von Winterfeldt, 2015), human judgment (Kruglanski and Ajzen, 1983), and strategic planning (Barnes, 1984). The origins of these biases can be traced to both innate and acquired factors, as well as to environmental influences (Croskerry et al., 2013). Despite the prevalence of these biases, individuals often fail to recognize them in their own decision-making (Pronin, 2007). Additionally, decision-makers can fall into psychological traps such as the anchoring trap and the status quo trap (Hammond et al., 1998).
The papers in this review include a wide range of factors that potentially affect perceptions of risk or skill and/or decisions, like overconfidence (e.g., Bonini et al., 2019), heuristic traps (e.g., Furman et al., 2010), availability affect (e.g., Mannberg et al., 2021a), or framing effects (e.g., Stephensen et al., 2021b) and also theoretical (e.g., Zajchowski et al., 2016) and environmental factors (e.g., Wickens et al., 2015). Existing studies in this category typically investigate whether people make biased judgments and/or how biases and heuristics affect decision-making in avalanche terrain.
3.1.2 Risk communication (N=9)
Risk communication is a critical aspect of informing the public about potential risks, particularly in public health emergencies (Glik, 2007; Wachinger et al., 2012), and has an impact on risk perception and decision-making (Williams and Noyes, 2007). However, it is often challenging due to the complexity of risk information and the need to consider and understand the audience's beliefs, values, and concerns (Fischhoff, 2015; Keeney and von Winterfeldt, 1986). The presentation of risk information can significantly impact its effectiveness, with visual aids such as graphics playing a key role (Lipkus and Hollands, 1999).
Within the avalanche context, the tag mainly concerns communication via avalanche bulletins. Existing studies in this category cover both how different groups use and understand the content in avalanche bulletins (e.g., Fisher et al., 2022) and how the presentation of the information aids or hampers understanding (e.g., Engeset et al., 2018).
3.1.3 Avalanche education (N=4)
Education plays a crucial role in the ability to conduct risk management in uncertain environments (Ciocoiu and Neicu, 2007). Education may also help in understanding risk and uncertainty (Manson, 2018; Stalker, 2003). The effect of education is pivotal, especially in activities that take place in complex and wicked environments, where potentially fatal situations are a possibility.
Two of the four existing studies discuss the role of heuristic traps in avalanche courses (Johnson et al., 2020; Zajchowski et al., 2016). The third study concerns how the processing skills of avalanche bulletin information vary among recreationists and how this can be an avenue for continuing education (Fisher et al., 2022). The fourth study evaluates the effect of avalanche education on risk perception (Greene et al., 2022). It should be mentioned that many studies use avalanche education as one of many control variables, but these studies are not included under this tag. The four papers in this category do not cover the effects of avalanche education on knowledge and skills and analyses of the structure and content of avalanche courses.
3.1.4 Experience (N=2)
Experience can build expertise and therefore significantly impact risk management, but the role of experience in the risk identification process is much less significant than it is commonly assumed to be (Maytorena et al., 2007). Particularly, in wicked learning environments where feedback is sparse, experience does not necessarily lead to expertise (Hogarth et al., 2015).
There are only two papers in this category. One of the studies proposes a new way of measuring expertise (Stewart-Patterson, 2016). The other investigates how skill affects assessments and understanding of avalanche risk (Hallandvik et al., 2017). However, several other papers have this as an auxiliary concept; e.g., Landrø et al. (2020a) studies experts' decision-making.
3.1.5 Risk perception (N=10)
Risk perception is a complex phenomenon influenced by various factors and covers both the perceived likelihood of an outcome and how dangerous the outcome is perceived to be. Humans have a poor understanding of probabilities (Hertwig and Erev, 2009). Several studies highlight the role of emotions and cognitive processes in shaping risk perception (Slovic, 1987; Slovic et al., 2007). Other contributing factors are personal experiences and cultural factors (Hicks and Brown, 2013; Wachinger et al., 2012) and attitude, risk sensitivity, and specific fear (Joffe, 2003; Sjöberg, 2000).
In the avalanche literature, studies have focused on a variety of factors that impact risk perception like impact from experience of fatal avalanche events (e.g., Leiter, 2011), the cognitive effect of framing (e.g., Stephensen et al., 2021b), physical effects of activity (e.g., Raue et al., 2017), the effect of travel strategies (e.g., Michaelsen et al., 2022), or the impact of online user platforms (e.g., Plank, 2016).
3.1.6 Willingness to take risk (N=10)
While risk perception describes a person's understanding of how likely or dangerous a situation is, risk preferences, or willingness to take risk, describe how much they like or dislike the situation given the perceived risk (Dohmen et al., 2011; Pratt, 1978). Willingness to take risk is tied to demographic factors like gender, age, height, and parental background (Dohmen et al., 2011); individual factors like sensation seeking (Sharifpour et al., 2013), risk conception, and positive feelings (Dohmen et al., 2018; Isen and Patrick, 1983); social factors like influence from peers and mortality salience (Hirschberger et al., 2002; Woodside, 1972); and external factors (Hetschko and Preuss, 2020; Savage, 1993).
Existing studies in this category typically study how risk preferences correlate with decisions (e.g., Haegeli et al., 2012; Mannberg et al., 2018) or how willingness to take risk correlates with participant characteristics like gender and age (e.g., Mannberg et al., 2018; Walker and Latosuo, 2016) or cohort (e.g., Haegeli et al., 2012; Kopp et al., 2016) or external factors like equipment (e.g., Haegeli et al., 2014).
3.1.7 Social factors and group decision-making (N=6)
Being in a group affects performance and decision-making in multiple ways (Kerr and Tindale, 2004). A group will often outperform individual decision-makers (Kugler et al., 2012; Malone and Bernstein, 2022). However, negative group factors have been repeatedly shown to decrease decision quality (Kroon et al., 1991) and lead to higher risk-taking (Bougheas et al., 2013) and can lead to fatally flawed decisions (Sunstein and Hastie, 2008). Group size has been shown to be an important predictor, where large groups can lead to riskier decisions and challenge communication within groups with groups perhaps only discussing already-shared information and holding back information that is only known to parts of the group (Stasser and Titus, 1985).
Studies in this category include formation, leadership, and decision-making in groups (e.g., Zweifel and Haegeli, 2014); social aspiration (e.g., Mannberg et al., 2021b); moral boundaries (Tøstesen and Langseth, 2021); group size (Zweifel et al., 2016); organizational culture (Johnson et al., 2016); and decision-making within groups and how groups affect the decisions made by individuals (e.g., Ebert and Morreau, 2023). There is a large spread in the focus of existing studies. Topics include group formation; how group size, composition, decision rules affect the quality of decisions; and how organizational and social norms affect behavior.
3.1.8 Avalanche accidents (N=10)
Accident studies in general offer valuable insights into the causes and prevention of accidents and provide opportunities for learning (Balasubramanian and Louvar, 2002; Hovden et al., 2011). However, accidents are complex phenomena which benefit from a comprehensive approach (Cedergren and Petersen, 2011; Moura et al., 2017). Yet, feedback from experience and accidents is important for improving operational security (Croft, 2020; Lindberg et al., 2010).
Studies in this category include trends in accident rates (e.g., Berlin et al., 2019; Page et al., 1999), correlates of avalanche accidents and demographic factors (e.g., Jekich et al., 2016; Peitzsch et al., 2020), victim profiles (e.g., Soule et al., 2017), group size (e.g., Zweifel et al., 2016), fatality risk in helicopter and snow cat skiing (Walcher et al., 2019), and organizational culture (Johnson et al., 2016). The existing studies typically characterize avalanche victims or the situation leading up to the accident.
3.1.9 Population characteristics (N=11)
People traveling in avalanche terrain are not one homogeneous group but rather a heterogeneous collection of people with different motives, skills, and ways and means of travel. Tailoring risk mitigation strategies to specific user groups is crucial for their effectiveness (Bartolucci et al., 2023).
This concept is broad. It includes studies that in some way characterize a “population”, regardless of size. Studies in this category present characteristics for different populations in terms of safety practices (Nichols et al., 2018; Silverton et al., 2007, 2009), use of avalanche safety equipment (e.g., Ng et al., 2015), and a broader focus on human factors and motivation among different groups (Jackman et al., 2023; Sole et al., 2010).
3.1.10 Decision-making strategies (N=17)
Decision-making in the face of uncertainty is a complex process that requires a range of strategies (Reale et al., 2023). These strategies can take many forms, from predefined (rule-based) strategies to heuristics (Gigerenzer and Gaissmaier, 2011) and routines or vaguely defined habits (Verplanken and Aarts, 1999; Løland and Hällgren, 2025). In the decision-making process, the decision-makers need to consider a wide range of potential states and outcomes, as well as the reliability of information (Hansson, 1996; Polasky et al., 2011). Coping with such uncertainty requires mental preparedness, agility, and the ability to react to unforeseen events (Kleindorfer, 2008).
The existing literature on decision-making strategies has a very large spread concerning both method and focus. The studies typically either describe or test relevant strategies, underlying decision-making factors, or the use of decision-making aids in different user groups.
The 17 papers cover both methodological procedures (e.g., Sterchi and Haegeli, 2019; Thumlert and Haegeli, 2017) and empirical data collected on human behavior and mitigation strategies in avalanche terrain (e.g., Michaelsen et al., 2022). The literature spans investigations of professionals (e.g., Løland and Hällgren, 2023) and recreationists (e.g., Grimsdottir and McClung, 2006) and covers research on decision-making strategies of backcountry skiers (e.g., Pfeifer, 2009; Witting et al., 2021), skiers engaged in mechanized skiing (e.g., Hendrikx and Johnson, 2016; Sterchi and Haegeli, 2019), and snowmobilers (e.g., Baker and McGee, 2016; Michaelsen et al., 2022).
3.1.11 Motivation (N=3)
Motivation potentially affects a wide range of factors that drive risk exposure (Kerr and Houge Mackenzie, 2012) and engagement in analytical thinking (Mækelæ et al., 2023). In the avalanche context, this relates to terrain choices, educational choices, information search, use of risk mitigation strategies, etc.
The concept covers studies that describe either motivational factors in different user groups (Frühauf et al., 2019) or how motivations affect decision-making. The three existing papers in this category focus mainly on motives for seeking risk among lift-assisted skiers (Frühauf et al., 2017, 2019, 2020).
3.1.12 Methods and theory (N=7)
The field of social science is characterized by a broad but important variety of theories and methods (Della Porta and Keating, 2008). Examples of methods can be observation studies, interviews, surveys, and experiments, each with their own strengths and limitations (Herzog, 1996). It is therefore important to consider the specific research problem and context when choosing what methodological tools to apply.
The existing studies include papers that develop and describe a new theory or a new empirical method to collect or analyze data that can help in gaining a better understanding of human factors in avalanche terrain.
Several of the existing papers in this category present methods for GPS tracking in combination with surveys to collect data on terrain use and travel behavior in recreational out-of-bounds skiing (Johnson and Hendrikx, 2021; Sykes et al., 2020). Further, this concept covers methodological investigations to document terrain preferences (Saly et al., 2020) and terrain selection practices (Thumlert and Haegeli, 2017).
In Table 4 the different papers from all 12 research themes are presented with their different theme tags.
Our review shows that the number of peer-reviewed papers on the human factors in avalanche decision-making has increased substantially during the past decade. The vast majority of published studies use convenience sample methods to collect data and quantitative methods to analyze data from their participants, who mainly consist of recreational backcountry users (especially skiers). In this study we only include papers describing how human factors influence actual decision-making or risk assessment for those exposed to avalanche risk. However, there are a number of related topics that should also be explored, like avalanche rescue and medical issues and technology or solutions to assist decisions or mitigate avalanche risk including, among many other aspects, avalanche forecasting, management, and decision-making in operations where the decision-maker is not personally affected.
Our review of research themes suggests that most papers have research questions related to “biases and decision-making errors” (N=11), “risk communication” (N=9), “risk perception” (N=10), or “willingness to take risk” (N=10). Many of the papers provide descriptions of the behaviors or characteristics of specific groups of backcountry users. These papers were often categorized as “population characteristics” (N=11) or “decision-making strategies” (N=17). However, we would like to highlight that, given the large variety of studies included, the two latter research themes are broader and thus less informative than the other themes.
Within each category there are gaps and interesting questions for future studies. The studies within each category could have been explored in more detail, for example, through narrative reviews, and compared to studies beyond the avalanche literature through gap analysis. This is beyond the scope of this study but a worthwhile effort for future studies. We do however note that the literature on important topics like social factors (N=6), motivation (N=3), experience (N=2), and avalanche education (N=4) is very limited and therefore not suitable for narrative reviews. We therefore would like to make some comments on why these are important potential questions to ask and some reflections on how to approach them.
4.1 Social factors and group decision-making
Most decisions are made by groups and not individuals. This is especially the case for recreational decision-making in avalanche terrain. The sociality of humans further means that our decisions are very susceptible to the influence of people around us, and this affects decision-making in multiple ways (Kerr and Tindale, 2004).
At their best, groups can easily outsmart individuals (Malone and Bernstein, 2022). However, individuals within groups are subject to a number of dynamics that influence decision-making beyond their immediate control. These dynamics can lead them into pitfalls and dilemmas that could potentially be mitigated with greater knowledge and awareness of typical social mechanisms present in groups navigating avalanche terrain. At their worst, groups can have detrimental or even catastrophic effects on decision-making (Cartwright, 1973; Hart, 1991). Determining factors include group size and composition, formation and leadership, communication and skill, social aspiration, culture and morals, and cohesion and trust. Only a few of these topics have received attention in avalanche literature, and many important questions remain unexplored.
4.2 Motivation
Motivation affects a wide range of behaviors that can propel people to search for information or use products and services designed to improve their decisions. However, people have different motives for the same activity (Hornby et al., 2024). This variability suggests that motivation is not only a driver of behavior but also a potential source of bias, especially when strong motivation leads to an overshadowing or underestimation of cumulative risks, as observed in contexts involving appealing or high-stakes outcomes (Knäuper et al., 2005). Such motivational biases can result in individuals disregarding potential risks or rationalizing behavior that may compromise long-term well-being. In this study we only found three papers that specifically focus on motivation, and even here the focus is more toward slopes and freeriding. An investigation of motives for different segments of backcountry skiers, maybe separating between genders, terrain choices, or locals vs. tourists, is warranted. A systematic review study on motivation in extreme sport (Hornby et al., 2024) found that the more self-efficacy people had in their activity, the more risk they were willing to take. However, unlike many other sports, the major hazards of avalanches are not directly tied to mastering skiing, and the dynamics of self-efficacy in particular or of motivation more generally may be different than in other risk-prone activities.
4.3 Experience
In an environment with high-quality feedback, experience may translate into expertise (Ericsson, 2008). This is unfortunately not the case in avalanche terrain. The inherent lack of feedback creates a wicked learning environment (Hogarth et al., 2015). In addition, avalanche assessments are complex, even for trained experts (Landrø et al., 2020b), and without first-hand experience of avalanche accidents the risk is abstract (Hetland et al., 2024), leaving fear to be among the least prominent emotions among skiers (Hetland et al., 2018).
As in many other fields, the absence of catastrophic events often presents a unique challenge for accurately assessing risk and guiding future actions. While an avalanche provides clear feedback that informs risk perception and promotes preventative measures, the lack of such an event can lead to cognitive biases and distorted risk assessments. This phenomenon, sometimes described as “the dog that didn't bark”, occurs when individuals or societies overlook potential risks because they have not recently experienced adverse events (Kahneman and Tversky, 2013). The role of experience is therefore important in order to understand how the absence of avalanche events can lead to complacency, overconfidence, and behavior based on perceived, rather than actual, risk levels (Stephensen et al., 2021a). The two studies presented in this review provide a first take on how to assess expertise decoupled from experience (Stewart-Patterson, 2016) and the role of experience and behavioral consequences across skill levels (Hallandvik et al., 2017). Understanding how decision-makers interpret – or ignore – the absence of negative feedback is essential for developing frameworks that ensure effective education or risk management or promote sustainable behaviors in the face of low-probability, high-impact events like avalanches.
4.4 Avalanche education
Avalanche education provided by trained instructors ideally leads to improved skills in risk assessment and mitigation. However, we have not found any papers analyzing the quality of avalanche education or how courses can be improved to increase learning. The studies in this review underscore that decision-making in avalanche terrain is a complex process with many moving parts in uncertain environments where feedback is fickle. However, when people are most often the cause and victims of injury and death in avalanche terrain, the crux of the problem is avalanche education. How do people come to understand and later manage those complex factors? To date, avalanche education research has sorely lacked careful studies of how people are taught and learn relevant knowledge and skills and how people keep their knowledge and skills current. What knowledge and skills are essential and when? Which ways of learning are most effective, and how do they work? How is effective avalanche education made readily available to those who need it, and how do we assure that they get it for not only their own safety but also the safety of others? How does avalanche education change behavior? And does avalanche education leave people less exposed to risk or in fact make people more susceptible to exposing themselves to a risk they may not fully appreciate? (Yudkowsky, 2008). These questions deserve urgent interdisciplinary research attention.
4.5 Methodological approaches
Most of the papers included in this scoping review rely on a quantitative analysis of cross-sectional convenience samples; i.e., participants are recruited via personal networks, social media, or avalanche organizations and are only observed once. Most studies extract information via surveys. While these kinds of analyses can increase our understanding of some factors that affect decisions in avalanche terrain, the conclusions that can be drawn from the analyses are limited. There are several reasons for this.
Using convenience sampling via “avalanche networks” means that the researcher is more likely to reach participants with some form of interest in avalanche safety (e.g., visiting the avalanche bulletin website). In addition, among the individuals reached, those with a greater interest in avalanche safety are more likely to complete their participation. Since both learning and decision-making likely depend on interest, results from studies relying on convenience samples may not hold for the general population at risk of avalanches.
Non-experimental cross-sectional analysis can identify correlations between different factors (e.g., avalanche education/avalanche bulletin use and avalanche accidents) but cannot identify causal mechanisms or the direction of causation. There are several reasons for this, one of which is self-selection. As with participation in research studies, participation in avalanche courses and reading the avalanche bulletin likely covary with the interest in venturing into avalanche terrain (or with avalanche safety). In other words, finding that avalanche training/reading the bulletin correlates with experience of avalanche incidents or terrain choices is not sufficient to draw the conclusion that courses or forecasts have a causal effect on risk exposure. Experimental studies randomly assign participants to different “treatments” (participating in a course, reading the bulletin). As such, these studies avoid the selection problems described above. Non-experimental longitudinal studies (studies that follow people over time) have issues with self-selection but can evaluate changes in behavior and preferences before and after an event. This makes it possible to identify causal effects on a specific group of participants, even if it is not possible to generalize the results to the general population.
Finally, surveys that ask participants about their stated preferences and experiences can elicit information about what people think they would feel and do in different situations or what they remember from past situations. However, people in general are poor at predicting how they will feel and act in situations that are different from their current one (Mathews and Bradle, 1983; Thomas and Diener, 1990). In addition, humans' need to preserve a positive self-image can affect how we remember and explain past experiences (e.g., Alicke and Sedikides, 2009). In situ studies, which observe participants in the field when the experiences occur, therefore hold potential to reveal mechanisms that surveys fail to find.
4.6 Limitations
The spreadsheet containing the data from eligible papers has some limitations that should be kept in mind when used. First, to systematically assign a main concept to a paper, we focused on the paper's primary objective and focal research question. However, human factors in avalanche decision-making are a complex concept, and a single paper can encompass insights relevant to a multitude of topics. In addition, while all included studies are published peer-reviewed studies, the clarity of the research question and the link between the research question and analysis vary substantially in the final dataset. The resulting concepts may therefore provide an overly simplistic picture of the content in the current literature. Much of the literature offers insights that extend to topics beyond their main concept, and the resulting categorization should not be considered a measure of topic inclusion.
Second, while the data extraction and organization of the material followed a structured procedure, the evaluation was performed by a limited number of researchers. This means that the papers have been interpreted through the lens of a few individuals. The evaluation is therefore subjective, and other researchers may have categorized the data differently.
Finally, the methodological decisions relating to the eligibility criteria, publication status, years, and languages considered and information sources for the literature were aimed to create a more systematic review. While these decisions improved the relevance, consistency, and quality of the studies, they have drawbacks in that they inherently create a publication bias. As a result, the current study is biased toward Western academic perspectives in predominantly European and North American industry contexts. However, given that this study is a first attempt to consolidate this body of research from across the widely dispersed and inconsistent publishing outlets utilized by the avalanche community, it serves as a fundamental first step toward subsequently building more comprehensive and inclusive overviews of the literature.
The aim of the systematic literature search was to provide an overview of the existing body of research on human factors in avalanche decision-making. We hope the shared spreadsheet and the organization of the literature into different research themes will help researchers find relevant literature and identify important knowledge gaps that remain to be filled.
We would like to end with a call to action. The work with this literature search has been challenging for mainly two reasons. First, many papers lack clear and relevant keywords. This made it difficult to identify them in our search. Second, some of the papers proved difficult to access, even after trying to contact authors or libraries. We therefore envision a shared database similar to PsycInfo with the categorization of studies in various categories, and we encourage authors to publish their papers in open-access format so that important messages are not locked behind paywalls. This is particularly important given that the readership may be practitioners without access to scientific libraries. Finally, we encourage researchers within the field to draw attention to existing gaps that should be closed, where assessing the quality of avalanche education is most compelling.
All relevant data for this study can be downloaded at https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/U9YDM (Hetland, 2023).
AH led the project and has been involved in all stages of the project including design, implementation, and writing and editing paper. RAH designed and ran the search, developed the sorting procedure, and was involved in writing and editing, TTS finalized sorting and was involved in writing and editing. AM gave advice regarding design and implementation and was involved in writing and editing.
The contact author has declared that none of the authors has any competing interests.
Publisher's note: Copernicus Publications remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims made in the text, published maps, institutional affiliations, or any other geographical representation in this paper. While Copernicus Publications makes every effort to include appropriate place names, the final responsibility lies with the authors.
This article is part of the special issue “Latest developments in snow science and avalanche risk management research – merging theory and practice”. It is a result of the International snow science Workshop, Bend, Oregon, USA, 8–13 October 2023.
We would like to thank everyone that has contributed to the sorting process, particularly Finn Hovem, Ingrid Stette Haaberg, and Markus Aase for their extended and laborious effort in the initial screening. We would also like to thank our three international collaborators Pascal Haegeli, Ann St. Clair, and Kelly McNeil for valuable help in identifying the concepts and contributing to the proceedings paper version of this article that was presented at ISSW in Bend, October 2023.
This research has been supported by NordForsk (grant no. 105061).
This paper was edited by Yves Bühler and reviewed by Benjamin Zweifel and one anonymous referee.
Alicke, M. D. and Sedikides, C.: Self-enhancement and self-protection: What they are and what they do, Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol., 20, 1–48, https://doi.org/10.1080/10463280802613866, 2009.
Anders Ericsson, K.: Deliberate practice and acquisition of expert performance: a general overview, Acad. Emerg. Med., 15, 988–994, 2008.
Baker, J. and McGee, T. K.: Backcountry snowmobilers' avalanche-related information-seeking and preparedness behaviors, Soc. Nat. Resour., 29, 345–356, https://doi.org/10.1080/08941920.2015.1103387, 2016.
Balasubramanian, S. G. and Louvar, J. F.: Study of major accidents and lessons learned, Process Saf. Prog., 21, 237–244, 2002.
Barnes Jr., J. H.: Cognitive biases and their impact on strategic planning, Strateg. Manage. J., 5, 129–137, 1984.
Bartolucci, A., Aquilino, M. C., Bril, L., Duncan, J., and van Steen, T.: Effectiveness of audience segmentation in instructional risk communication: A systematic literature review, Int. J. Disast. Risk Re., 95, 103872, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijdrr.2023.103872, 2023.
Berlin, C., Techel, F., Moor, B. K., Zwahlen, M., Hasler, R. M., and Swiss National Cohort study group: Snow avalanche deaths in Switzerland from 1995 to 2014 – Results of a nation-wide linkage study, PLoS One, 14, e0225735, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0225735, 2019.
Bonini, N., Pighin, S., Rettore, E., Savadori, L., Schena, F., Tonini, S., and Tosi, P.: Overconfident people are more exposed to “black swan” events: a case study of avalanche risk, Empir. Econ., 57, 1443–1467, https://doi.org/10.1007/s00181-018-1489-5, 2019.
Bougheas, S., Nieboer, J., and Sefton, M.: Risk-taking in social settings: Group and peer effects, J. Econ. Behav. Organ., 92, 273–283, 2013.
Cartwright, D.: Determinants of scientific progress: The case of research on the risky shift, Am. Psychol., 28, 222, https://doi.org/10.1037/h0034445, 1973.
Cedergren, A. and Petersen, K.: Prerequisites for learning from accident investigations – A cross-country comparison of national accident investigation boards, Safety Sci., 49, 1238–1245, 2011.
Ciocoiu, C. N. and Neicu, D.: Education and Professional Training in the Field of Risk Management, Economia Seria Management, 10, 12–26, 2007.
Croft, J.: The Lessons to be Learned from Incidents and Accidents, in: Current trends in radiation protection, EDP Sciences, 149–164, https://doi.org/10.1051/978-2-86883-725-7.c015, 2020.
Croskerry, P., Singhal, G., and Mamede, S. ilvia: Cognitive debiasing 1: origins of bias and theory of debiasing, BMJ Qual. Saf., 22, ii58–ii64, 2013.
Della Porta, D. and Keating, M. (Eds.): Approaches and methodologies in the social sciences: A pluralist perspective, Cambridge University Press, ISBN 9780511801938, https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511801938, 2008.
Dohmen, T., Falk, A., Huffman, D., Sunde, U., Schupp, J., and Wagner, G. G.: Individual risk attitudes: Measurement, determinants, and behavioral consequences, J. Eur. Econ. Assoc., 9, 522–550, 2011.
Dohmen, T. Quercia, S., and Willrodt, J.: Willingness to Take Risk: The Role of Risk Conception and Optimism, IZA Discussion Paper No. 11642, https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3209739, 2018.
Ebert, P. A. and Morreau, M.: Safety in numbers: how social choice theory can inform avalanche risk management, Journal of Adventure Education and Outdoor Learning, 23, 340–356, 2023.
Engeset, R. V., Pfuhl, G., Landrø, M., Mannberg, A., and Hetland, A.: Communicating public avalanche warnings – what works?, Nat. Hazard. Earth Sys., 18, 2537–2559, https://doi.org/10.5194/nhess-18-2537-2018, 2018.
Fischhoff, B.: Risk perception and communication, in: Oxford Textbook of Global Public Health, Oxford University Press, 893–906, ISBN 9780203140710, 2015.
Fisher, K. C., Haegeli, P., and Mair, P.: Exploring the avalanche bulletin as an avenue for continuing education by including learning interventions, Journal of Outdoor Recreation and Tourism, 37, 100472, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jort.2021.100472, 2022.
Fruhaüf, A., Hardy, W. A., Pfoestl, D., Hoellen, F.-G., and Kopp, M.: A qualitative approach on motives and aspects of risks in freeriding, Front. Psychol., 8, 1998, https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01998, 2017.
Frühauf, A., Anewanter, P., Hagenauer, J., Marterer, N., and Kopp, M.: Freeriding – Only a need for thrill?: Comparing different motives and behavioural aspects between slope skiers and freeride skiers, J. Sci. Med. Sport, 22, S44–S49, 2019.
Frühauf, A., Zenzmaier, J., and Kopp, M.: Does age matter? A qualitative comparison of motives and aspects of risk in adolescent and adult freeriders, J. Sports Sci. Med., 19, 112–120, 2020.
Furman, N., Shooter, W., and Schumann, S.: The roles of heuristics, avalanche forecast, and risk propensity in the decision making of backcountry skiers, Leisure Sci., 32, 453–469, 2010.
Gigerenzer, G. and Gaissmaier, W.: Heuristic decision making, Annu. Rev. Psychol., 62, 451–482, 2011.
Glik, D. C.: Risk Communication for Public Health Emergencies, Annu. Rev. Publ. Health, 28, 33–54, 2007.
Greene, K., Hendrikx, J., and Johnson, J.: The impact of avalanche education on risk perception, confidence, and decision-making among backcountry skiers, Leisure Sci., 47, 113–133, https://doi.org/10.1080/01490400.2022.2062075, 2022.
Grimsdottir, H. and McClung, D.: Avalanche risk during backcountry skiing – An analysis of risk factors, Nat. Hazards, 39, 127–153, https://doi.org/10.1007/s11069-005-5227-x, 2006.
Haegeli, P., Gunn, M., and Haider, W.: Identifying a High-Risk Cohort in a Complex and Dynamic Risk Environment: Out-of-bounds Skiing – An Example from Avalanche Safety, Prev. Sci., 13, 562–573, https://doi.org/10.1007/s11121-012-0282-5, 2012.
Haegeli, P., Falk, M., Procter, E., Zweifel, B., Jarry, F., Logan, S., Kronholm, K., Biskupiè, M., and Brugger, H.: The effectiveness of avalanche airbags, Resuscitation, 85, 1197–1203, 2014.
Haegeli, P., Clair, A. S., McNeil, K., Mannberg, A., and Hetland, A.: Reflections on How to Improve the Contribution of Social Science Research to Avalanche Safety Practices, in: Proceedings of the 2023 International Snow Science Workshop, October 2023, Banf, USA, 8–13, https://arc.lib.montana.edu/snow-science/objects/ISSW2023_O3.01.pdf (last access: 12 February 2025), 2023.
Hallandvik, L., Andresen, M. S., and Aadland, E.: Decision-making in avalanche terrain–How does assessment of terrain, reading of avalanche forecast and environmental observations differ by skiers' skill level?, Journal of Outdoor Recreation and Tourism, 20, 45–51, 2017.
Hammond, J. S., Keeney, R. L., and Raiffa, H.: The hidden traps in decision making, Harvard Business Rev., 76, 47–58, 1998.
Hart, P.: Irving L. Janis' victims of groupthink, Polit. Psychol., 12, 247–278., https://doi.org/10.2307/3791464, 1991.
Hertwig, R. and Erev, I.: The description–experience gap in risky choice, Trends Cogn. Sci., 13, 517–523, 2009.
Herzog, T.: Research Methods in the Social Sciences, Harper Collins, New York, NY, ISBN 0673991067, 1996.
Hetland, A.: Literature review, OSF [data set], https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/U9YDM, 2024.
Hetland, A., Vittersø, J., Wie, S. O. B., Kjelstrup, E., Mittner, M., and Dahl, T. I.: Skiing and thinking about it: moment-to-moment and retrospective analysis of emotions in an extreme sport, Front. Psychol., 9, 971, https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00971, 2018.
Hetland, A., Skille, T. T., Mannberg, A., Kristensen, T., and Hjelm, I.: Buried alive: A qualitative study of avalanche survivors' learning experience after an avalanche accident, in: International Snow Science Workshop, September 2024, Tromsø, Norway, https://arc.lib.montana.edu/snow-science/objects/ISSW2024_O11.8.pdf (last access: 12 February 2025), 2024.
Hetschko, C. and Preuss, M.: Income in jeopardy: How losing employment affects the willingness to take risks, J. Econ. Psychol., 79, 102175, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2019.05.005, 2020.ÿ
Hicks, S. and Brown, S.: Perceptions of risk, International Review of Victimology, 19, 249–267, 2013.
Hirschberger, G., Florian, V., Mikulincer, M., Goldenberg, J. L., and Pyszczynski, T.: Gender differences in the willingness to engage in risky behavior: a terror management perspective, Death Stud., 26, 117–141, 2002.
Hogarth, R. M., Lejarraga, T., and Soyer, E.: The two settings of kind and wicked learning environments, Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci., 24, 379–385, 2015.
Hornby, O., Roderique-Davies, G., Heirene, R., Thorkildsen, E., Bradbury, S., Rowlands, I., Goodison, E., Gill, J., and Shearer, D.: What factors explain extreme sport participation? A systematic review, Frontiers in Sports and Active Living, 6, 1403499, https://doi.org/10.3389/fspor.2024.1403499, 2024.
Hovden, J., Størseth, F., and Tinmannsvik, R. K.: Multilevel learning from accidents – Case studies in transport, Safety Sci., 49, 98–105, 2011.
Isen, A. M. and Patrick, R.: The effect of positive feelings on risk taking: When the chips are down, Organ. Behav. Hum. Perf., 31, 194–202, 1983.
Jackman, P. C., Hawkins, R. M., Burke, S. M., Swann, C., and Crust, L.: The psychology of mountaineering: a systematic review, Int. Rev. Sport Exerc. Psychol., 16, 27–65, 2023.
Jekich, B. M., Drake, B. D., Nacht, J. Y., Nichols, A., Ginde, A. A., and Davis, C. B.: Avalanche Fatalities in the United States: A Change in Demographics, Wilderness Environ. Med., 27, 46–52, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wem.2015.11.004, 2016.
Joffe, H.: Risk: From perception to social representation, Br. J. Soc. Psychol., 42, 55–73, 2003.
Johnson, J, and Hendrikx, J.: Using Citizen Science to Document Terrain Use and Decision-Making of Backcountry Users, Citizen Science: Theory and Practice, 6, 8, https://doi.org/10.5334/cstp.333, 2021.
Johnson, J., Haegeli, P., Hendrikx, J., and Savage, S.: Accident causes and organizational culture among avalanche professionals, Journal of Outdoor Recreation and Tourism, 13, 49–56, 2016.
Johnson, J., Mannberg, A., Hendrikx, J., Hetland, A., and Stephensen, M.: Rethinking the heuristic traps paradigm in avalanche education: Past, present and future, Cogent Social Sciences, 6, 1807111, https://doi.org/10.1080/23311886.2020.1807111, 2020.
Kahneman, D. and Tversky, A.: Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk, in: Handbook of the fundamentals of financial decision making: Part I, World Scientific, 99–127, https://doi.org/10.2307/1914185, 2013.
Keeney, R. L. and von Winterfeldt, D.: Improving Risk Communication, Risk Anal., 6, 417–424, 1986.
Kerr, J. H. and Houge Mackenzie, S.: Multiple motives for participating in adventure sports, Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 13, 649–657, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2012.04.002, 2012.
Kerr, N. L. and Tindale, R. S.: Group performance and decision making, Annu. Rev. Psychol., 55, 623–655, 2004.
Kleindorfer, P. R.: Reflections on Decision Making Under Uncertainty, University of Pennsylvania, https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.1310239, 2008.
Knäuper, B., Kornik, R., Atkinson, K., Guberman, C., and Aydin, C.: Motivation influences the underestimation of cumulative risk, Pers. Soc. Psychol. B., 31, 1511–1523, 2005.
Kopp, M., Wolf, M., Ruedl, G., and Burtscher, M.: Differences in Sensation Seeking Between Alpine Skiers, Snowboarders and Ski Tourers, J. Sports Sci. Med., 15, 11–16, 2016.
Kroon, M. B., Hart, P., and Van Kreveld, D.: Managing group decision making processes: Individual versus collective accountability and groupthink, Int. J. Confl. Manage., 2, 91–115, 1991.
Kruglanski, A. W. and Ajzen, I.: Bias and error in human judgment, Eur. J. Soc. Psychol., 13, 1–44, 1983.
Kugler, T., Kausel, E. E., and Kocher, M. G.: Are groups more rational than individuals? A review of interactive decision making in groups, WiRes. Cogn. Sci., 3, 471–482, 2012.
Landrø, M., Hetland, A., Engeset, R. V., and Pfuhl, G.: Avalanche decision-making frameworks: Factors and methods used by experts, Cold Reg. Sci. Technol., 170, 102897, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.coldregions.2019.102897, 2020a.
Landrø, M., Pfuhl, G., Engeset, R., Jackson, M., and Hetland, A.: Avalanche decision-making frameworks: Classification and description of underlying factors, Cold Reg. Sci. Technol., 169, 102903, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.coldregions.2019.102903, 2020b.
Leiter, A. M.: The sense of snow–Individuals' perception of fatal avalanche events, J. Environ. Psychol., 31, 361–372, 2011.
Lindberg, A.-K., Hansson, S. O., and Rollenhagen, C.: Learning from accidents – What more do we need to know?, Safety Sci., 48, 714–721, 2010.
Lipkus, I. M. and Hollands, J. G.: The Visual Communication of Risk, JNCI-J. Natl. Cancer I., 1999, 149–163, 1999.
Løland, S. and Hällgren, M.: `Where to ski?': an ethnography of how guides make sense while planning, Leisure Stud., 42, 866–882, 2023.
Løland, S. and Hällgren, M.: Negotiating the gray zone: Ski guiding routine dynamics, Ann.f Tourism Res., 110, 103858, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.annals.2024.103858, 2025.
Mækelæ, M. J., Klevjer, K., Westbrook, A., Eby, N. S., Eriksen, R., and Pfuhl, G.: Is it cognitive effort you measure? Comparing three task paradigms to the Need for Cognition scale, PLoS One, 18, e0290177, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0290177, 2023.
Malone, T. W. and Bernstein, M. S.: Handbook of collective intelligence, MIT Press, ISBN 9780262545846, 2022.
Mannberg, A., Hendrikx, J., Landrø, M., and Stefan, M. A.: Who's at risk in the backcountry? Effects of individual characteristics on hypothetical terrain choices, J. Environ. Psychol., 59, 46–53, 2018.
Mannberg, A., Hendrikx, J., Johnson, J., and Hetland, A.: Powder fever and its impact on decision-making in avalanche terrain, Int. J. Env. Res. Pub. He., 18, 9496, https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18189496, 2021a.
Mannberg, A., Hendrikx, J., and Johnson, J.: Risky positioning–social aspirations and risk-taking behaviour in avalanche terrain, Leisure Stud., 40, 495–512, 2021b.
Manson, B.: Understanding risk in an emergency management context, J. Business Contin. Emerg. Plan., 12, 27–39, 2018.
Mathews, A. and Bradle, B.: Mood and the self-reference bias in recall, Behav. Res. Ther., 21, 233–239, https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(83)90204-8, 1983.
Maytorena, E., Winch, G. M., Freeman, J., and Kiely, T.: The Influence of Experience and Information Search Styles on Project Risk Identification Performance, IEEE T. Eng. Manage., 54, 315–326, 2007.
McCammon, I.: Evidence of heuristic traps in recreational avalanche accidents, in: 2002 International Snow Science Workshop, Penticton, BC, Canada, https://arc.lib.montana.edu/snow-science/item/837 (last access: 12 February 2025), 2002.
Michaelsen, B., Stewart-Patterson, I., Rolland, C. G., Hetland, A., and Engeset, R. V.: Behavior in avalanche terrain: an exploratory study of illegal snowmobiling in Norway, Int. J. Env. Res. Pub. He., 19, 6040, https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19106040, 2022.
Montibeller, G. and von Winterfeldt, D.: Cognitive and Motivational Biases in Decision and Risk Analysis, Risk Anal., 35, 1230–1251, 2015.
Moura, R., Beer, M., Patelli, E., Lewis, J., and Knoll, F.: Learning from accidents: Interactions between human factors, technology and organisations as a central element to validate risk studies, Safety Sci., 99, 196–214, 2017.
Ng, P., Smith, W. R., Wheeler, A., and McIntosh, S. E.: Advanced Avalanche Safety Equipment of Backcountry Users: Current Trends and Perceptions, Wilderness Environ. Med., 26, 417–421, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wem.2015.03.029, 2015.
Nichols, T. B., Hawley, A. C., Smith, W. R., Wheeler III, A. R., and McIntosh, S. E.: Avalanche Safety Practices Among Backcountry Skiers and Snowboarders in Jackson Hole in 2016, Wilderness Environ. Med., 29, 493–498, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wem.2018.05.004, 2018.
Ove Hansson, S.: Decision Making Under Great Uncertainty, Philos. Soc. Sci., 26, 369–386, 1996.
Page, C. E., Atkins, D., Shockley, L. W., and Yaron, M.: Avalanche deaths in the United States: a 45 year analysis, Wilderness Environ. Med., 10, 146–151, https://doi.org/10.1580/1080-6032(1999)010[0146:Aditus]2.3.Co;2, 1999.
Peitzsch, E., Boilen, S., Logan, S., Birkeland, K., and Greene, E.: Research note: How old are the people who die in avalanches? A look into the ages of avalanche victims in the United States (1950–2018), Journal of Outdoor Recreation and Tourism, 29, 100255, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jort.2019.100255, 2020.
Pfeifer, C.: On probabilities of avalanches triggered by alpine skiers. An empirically driven decision strategy for backcountry skiers based on these probabilities, Nat. Hazards, 48, 425–438, https://doi.org/10.1007/s11069-008-9270-2, 2009.
Plank, A.: The hidden risk in user-generated content: An investigation of ski tourers' revealed risk-taking behavior on an online outdoor sports platform, Tourism Manage., 55, 289–296, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tourman.2016.02.013, 2016.
Polasky, S., Carpenter, S. R., Folke, C., and Keeler, B.: Decision-making under great uncertainty: environmental management in an era of global change, Trends Ecol. Evol., 26, 398–404, 2011.
Pratt, J. W.: Risk aversion in the small and in the large, in: Uncertainty in economics, Elsevier, 59–79, https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-214850-7.50010-3, 1978.
Pronin, E.: Perception and misperception of bias in human judgment, Trends Cogn. Sci., 11, 37–43, 2007.
Raue, M., Streicher, B., Lermer, E., and Frey, D.: Being active when judging risks: bodily states interfere with accurate risk analysis, J. Risk Res., 20, 445–462, https://doi.org/10.1080/13669877.2015.1057206, 2017.
Reale, C., Salwei, M. E., Militello, L. G., Weinger, M. B., Burden, A., Sushereba, C., Torsher, L. C., Andreae, M. H., Gaba, D. M., and McIvor, W. R.: Decision-making during high-risk events: a systematic literature review, Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision Making, 17, 188–212, 2023.
Saly, D., Hendrikx, J., Birkeland, K. W., Challender, S., and Johnson, J.: Using time lapse photography to document terrain preferences of backcountry skiers, Cold Reg. Sci. Technol., 172, 102994, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.coldregions.2020.102994, 2020.
Savage, L.: An empirical investigation into the effect of psychological perceptions on the willingness-to-pay to reduce risk, J. Risk Uncertainty, 6, 75–90, 1993.
Schweizer, J. and Lütschg, M.: Characteristics of human-triggered avalanches, Cold Reg. Sci. Technol., 33, 147–162, 2001.
Sharifpour, M., Walters, G., and Ritchie, B. W.: The mediating role of sensation seeking on the relationship between risk perceptions and travel behavior, Tourism Analysis, 18, 543–557, 2013.
Silverton, N. A., McIntosh, S. E., and Kim, H. S.: Avalanche safety practices in Utah, Wilderness Environ. Med., 18, 264–270, https://doi.org/10.1580/06-weme-or-049r2.1, 2007.
Silverton, N. A., McIntosh, S. E., and Kim, H. S.: Risk Assessment in Winter Backcountry Travel, Wilderness Environ. Med., 20, 269–274, https://doi.org/10.1580/08-weme-or-209r1.1, 2009.
Sjöberg, L.: The methodology of risk perception research, Qual. Quant., 34, 407–418, 2000.
Sjöoberg, L.: Factors in Risk Perception, Risk Anal., 20, 1–12, 2000.
Slovic, P.: Perception of Risk, Science, 236, 280–285, 1987.
Slovic, P., Finucane, M. L., Peters, E., and MacGregor, D. G.: The affect heuristic, Eur. J. Oper. Res., 177, 1333–1352, 2007.
Sole, A. E., Emery, C. A., Hagel, B. E., and Morrongiello, B. A.: Risk Taking in Avalanche Terrain: A Study of the Human Factor Contribution, Clin. J. Sport Med., 20, 445–451, https://doi.org/10.1097/JSM.0b013e3181fc0a6d, 2010.
Soule, B., Reynier, V., Lefevre, B., and Boutroy, E.: Who is at risk in the French mountains? Profiles of the accident victims in outdoor sports and mountain recreation, J. Mt. Sci., 14, 1490–1499, 2017.
Stalker, K.: Managing Risk and Uncertainty in Social Work, J. Soc. Work, 3, 211–233, 2003.
Stasser, G. and Titus, W.: Pooling of unshared information in group decision making: Biased information sampling during discussion., J. Pers. Soc. Psychol., 48, 1467, https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.48.6.1467, 1985.
Stephensen, M. B., Martiny-Huenger, T., and Schulze, C.: Confidence in complex risk judgments: the roles of uncertainty, experience, and affect, OSF, https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/gk7xc, 2021a.
Stephensen, M. B., Schulze, C., Landrø, M., Hendrikx, J., and Hetland, A.: Should I judge safety or danger? Perceived risk depends on the question frame, J. Exp. Psychol.: Appl., 27, 485, https://doi.org/10.1037/xap0000354, 2021b.
Sterchi, R. and Haegeli, P.: A method of deriving operation-specific ski run classes for avalanche risk management decisions in mechanized skiing, Nat. Hazards Earth Syst. Sci., 19, 269–285, https://doi.org/10.5194/nhess-19-269-2019, 2019.
Stewart-Patterson, I.: Measuring decision expertise in commercial ski guiding in a more meaningful way, Journal of Outdoor Recreation and Tourism, 13, 44–48, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jort.2015.11.009, 2016.
Sunstein, C. R. and Hastie, R.: Four Failures of Deliberating Groups, The University of Chicago, https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.1121400, 2008.
Sykes, J., Hendrikx, J., Johnson, J., and Birkeland, K. W.: Combining GPS tracking and survey data to better understand travel behavior of out-of-bounds skiers, Appl. Geogr., 122, 102261, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apgeog.2020.102261, 2020.
Thomas, D. L. and Diener, E.: Memory accuracy in the recall of emotions, J. Pers. Soc. Psychol., 59, 291–297, https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.59.2.291, 1990.
Thumlert, S. and Haegeli, P.: Describing the severity of avalanche terrain numerically using the observed terrain selection practices of professional guides, Nat. Hazards, 91, 89–115, https://doi.org/10.1007/s11069-017-3113-y, 2017.
Tøstesen, G. and Langseth, T.: Freeride Skiing – risk-taking, recognition, and moral boundaries, Frontiers in Sports and Active Living, 3, 650564, https://doi.org/10.3389/fspor.2021.650564, 2021.
Tricco, A. C., Lillie, E., Zarin, W., O'Brien, K. K., Colquhoun, H., Levac, D., Moher, D., Peters, M. D., Horsley, T., and Weeks, L.: PRISMA extension for scoping reviews (PRISMA-ScR): checklist and explanation, Ann. Intern. Med., 169, 467–473, 2018.
Verplanken, B. and Aarts, H.: Habit, attitude, and planned behaviour: is habit an empty construct or an interesting case of goal-directed automaticity?, Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol., 10, 101–134, 1999.
Wachinger, G., Renn, O., Begg, C., and Kuhlicke, C.: The Risk Perception Paradox – Implications for Governance and Communication of Natural Hazards, Risk Anal., 33, 1049–1065, 2012.
Walcher, M., Haegeli, P., and Fuchs, S.: Risk of death and major injury from natural winter hazards in helicopter and snowcat skiing in Canada, Wilderness Environ. Med., 30, 251–259, 2019.
Walker, E. and Latosuo, E.: Gendered decision-making practices in Alaska's dynamic mountain environments? A study of professional mountain guides, Journal of Outdoor Recreation and Tourism, 13, 18–22, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jort.2015.11.010, 2016.
Wickens, C. D., Keller, J. W., and Shaw, C.: Human factors in high-altitude mountaineering, J. Hum. Perform. Ext. Environ., 12, 1–19, https://doi.org/10.7771/2327-2937.1065, 2015.
Williams, D. J. and Noyes, J. M.: How does our perception of risk influence decision-making? Implications for the design of risk information, Theoretical Issues in Ergonomics Science, 8, 1–35, 2007.
Witting, M., Filimon, S., and Kevork, S.: Carry along or not? Decision-making on carrying standard avalanche safety gear among ski tourers in a German touring region, Safety Sci., 143, 105406, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssci.2021.105406, 2021.
Woodside, A. G.: Informal Group Influence on Risk Taking, J. Marketing Res., 9, 223–225, 1972.
Yudkowsky, E.: Cognitive biases potentially affecting judgment of global risks, Global Catastrophic Risks, Oxford Academic, 91–119, ISBN 9780199606504, 2008.
Zajchowski, C. A. B., Brownlee, M. T. J., and Furman, N. N.: The Dialectical Utility of Heuristic Processing in Outdoor Adventure Education, Journal of Outdoor Recreation Education and Leadership, 8, 119–135, https://doi.org/10.18666/jorel-2016-v8-i2-7697, 2016.
Zweifel, B. and Haegeli, P.: A qualitative analysis of group formation, leadership and decision making in recreation groups traveling in avalanche terrain, Journal of Outdoor Recreation and Tourism, 5–6, 17–26, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jort.2014.03.001, 2014.
Zweifel, B., Procter, E., Techel, F., Strapazzon, G., and Boutellier, R.: Risk of Avalanche Involvement in Winter Backcountry Recreation: The Advantage of Small Groups, Wilderness Environ. Med., 27, 203–210, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wem.2015.12.001, 2016.